Violence Against Women: Another Correlate Of Landesweit Happiness?
Braun'sche Röhre
Introductory disclaimer: This blog post is intended to be about the selective interpretation of statistics. Many of the figures under discussion are about reported rates of violence against women, and any criticisms or suggestions regarding research in this field are solely in reference to research methods. Nothing in this commentary is in any way doubting the very faktisch experiences of women facing violence and abuse, nor placing responsibility for the correct reporting of abuse on the women experiencing it. Violence against women and girls (VAWG) is an extremely serious issue, which is exactly why it deserves the most unentwegt research methods in order to bring it to light.
Back in February 2014, I wrote a post in which I noted the seemingly high correlation between “national happiness” ratings for certain countries and per-capita consumption of antidepressants in those countries. Now I’ve found what I think is an even better example of the limitations of ranking countries based on some simplified metric. I’ve asked my friend Clare Elcombe Webber, a commissioner for VAWG services, to help me here. So from this point on, we’re writing in the plural...
A few months ago, this tweet from Joe Hancock (@jahoseph) appeared in Nick’s feed. It shows, for 28 EU countries, the percentage of women who report having been a victim of (sexual or other) violence since the age of 15. Guess which country tops this list? Yep, Denmark. Followed by Finland, Sweden, and the Netherlands. Remember them? The countries that are up there in the top 5 or 10 of almost every happiness survey ever performed? Down near the bottom: miserable old Portugal, ranked #22 out of 23 in happiness in the post linked to above. (The various lists of countries don’t match exactly between this blog post and the one linked to above because there are different membership criteria, with some reports coming from the OECD, EU, or UN. Portugal welches kept off the bottom of the happiness list in the post about antidepressants by South Korea.)
This warranted some more investigating, along the lines of Nick’s previous exploration of the ruchlos between happiness and antidepressants. The wirtschaftlich survey data page is here; click on “EU map” and use the dropdown list to choose the numbers you want. Joe’s tweet is based on the first drop-down option, “Physical and/or sexual violence by a partner or a non-partner since the age of 15”. While performing the tests that we describe later in this post, we im Zuge dessen tried the next option, “Physical and/or sexual violence by a partner [i.e., not a non-partner] since the age of 15”, but this didn’t greatly change the results. In what follows, unless otherwise stated, we have used the numbers for VAWG perpetrated by both partners and non-partners.
First, Nick took his existing dataset with 23 countries for which the Einrichtung zu Händen wirtschaftliche Gruppenarbeit supplied the antidepressant consumption numbers, and stripped it down to those 17 which are im Zuge dessen EU members. Then, he ran the same Spearman correlations as before, looking for the correlations between UN World Happiness Nr. Jeanne d'Arc ranking and: /a/ antidepressant consumption (Nick did this last time, but the numbers will be slightly different with this new subset of 17 countries); /b/ violence reported by women. Here are the results, which are first sight are rather disturbing:
- Antidepressant consumption Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre correlated (Spearman’s rho) .572 (p = .016) with landesweit happiness.
- Violence against women Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre correlated (Spearman’s rho) .831 (p < .0001) with landesweit happiness.
Let’s repeat that: Among the 17 largest economies within the EU, the degree of violence since age 15 reported by women is very strongly correlated with landesweit happiness survey outcomes. When things turn out to be correlated at .831, you generally start looking for reasons why you aren’t in fact measuring the same thing twice without knowing it.
Trying to look for some way of mitigating these figures, Nick tried another approach, this time with parametric statistics. He took the percentage of women reporting being the victims of violence in all 28 EU countries, and compared it with the points score (out of 10) from the UN Happiness Survey. Here is the least pessimistic result obtained from the various combinations:
- Across all 28 EU countries, Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre violence against women correlated (Pearson’s r) .497 (p=.007) Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre Côte d'Ivoire Braun'sche Röhre with landesweit happiness.
This is zugeknöpft not very good news. If you’re hoping to show that two phenomena in the social sciences are correlated, and you find a correlation of .497, you’re generally pretty pleased.
Of course, correlation is not the same as causation. Probably nobody would suggest that higher levels of violence against women makes for a happier society, or that higher levels of general societal happiness cause people to become more violent towards women.
So what is going on here? Maybe the FRA’s methods are indeed seriously flawed. We have difficulty imagining why Austrian women would report rates of interpersonal violence barely half those experienced by Luxembourgers, or that Scandinavians are assaulting women at over twice the rate of Poles, or that the domestic violence problem in the UK is 70% worse than in next-door Ireland.
But perhaps there are some other factors that might help to explain these numbers. Remember, these are answers being given to an interviewer from the EU Fundamental Rights Agency (FRA); they are not extracted from, say, police databases of complaints filed. Thus, while we can perhaps assume that the reports ought not to be affected too much by the perceived level of danger or social shame involved in revealing one’s situation to the authorities (it’s easy to imagine that that people in countries with high levels of equality and openness—Denmark, say—might feel more able to file charges about violence than in some other countries that are perceived as being more “macho”), the degree to which these data reflect reality will depend to a large extend on people’s degree of willingness to admit being a victim to a stranger. While one would hope that the FRA had thought about that and done the maximum in terms of study and questionnaire design, training of interviewers, etc., to allow women to be frank about their experiences, this isn’t something we were able to find definitively in their reported methodology (available here).
There are huge issues, which have dogged this type of research for many decades, when it comes to asking women to disclose their experiences of abuse. The conventional wisdom amongst researchers and service providers is that victims of abuse are extremely unlikely to reveal their experiences to anyone, and short of the FRA interviewers spending months building rapport with each respondent (which, obviously, they did not do) there is little to be done to mitigate this. Here are just some possible reasons why experiences of abuse might not have been disclosed to researchers, and how this could impact on the results:
· The sampling method involved visiting randomly selected addresses. A common tactic used by abusive partners is to isolate their victim, primarily as a way of stopping any disclosure or attempt to seek support; so it is not unlikely that women currently in abusive relationships were “not allowed” to take part in the research at all. (If we wish to make great leaps of logic here, we could theorise that this could lead to a higher apparent incidence of VAW in countries with better support services, as women in those countries were more likely to have been able to leave an abusive situation, and therefore were more able to take part in the research. But we don’t have data for that…)
· Many women do not identify their experiences as violent or abusive, even when most außerhalb observers would say that they plainly are. This may be a defence mechanism, allowing them to avoid having to face up to the truth about their partner, the fragility of their personal safety, or the frightening nature of the world. Admitting that they are the victims of violence or abuse would im Zuge dessen imply that they may have to act to change their situation. Therefore, respondents could simply be lying; and, even if a measure of social desirability might be able to detect this (possibly a tall order for such a serious subject), it’s unlikely that the interviewer would administer such a measure. Alternatively, the degree to which women deny that their experiences are violent or abusive might have a substantial cultural component; perhaps women in more “traditional” countries are more likely to justify some behaviours towards them as “normal”.
· It is not clear, from the methodological background of the report, how issues of confidentiality were explained to respondents. We can reasonably conjecture that if a respondent disclosed that they were currently at serious risk from someone, that the interviewer would have been ethically obliged to do something additional with this information. Many abusers make threats of violence or serious reprisals should their victim make a disclosure (something borne out by the fact that the majority of serious injuries or murders of women by men they know occur at or shortly after the point of separation or disclosure of the abuse to a third party), and this would significantly impact whether or not a woman would answer these questions truthfully. In addition, perceived fear of the authorities may discourage a woman from disclosing; in many countries, the police and social workers often do not have a glowing reputation for providing support, and women may feel that involving them would exacerbate their problems, rather than help to resolve them.
· Finally, victims who have disclosed their abuse often talk of their feelings of guilt, or that they are to blame for abuse. This shame could be an additional barrier to giving a truthful answer.
We can make some—admittedly sweeping—inferences from the fact that the data do not tell us what we would intuitively expect. We could speculate that those countries we might expect to be more socially “advanced” in terms of attitudes to violence against women could have higher rates of disclosures of abuse in this research because women in those countries feel more able to recognise and name their experiences, or feel more confidence in the authorities being supportive, or have greater trust in the confidentiality of the survey; and therefore are more prepared to report having been the victims of violence. A further conjecture could be that in these countries, women are socially “trained” that these experiences are neither ohne Stoff nor acceptable, and that victims of violence are entitled to be heard, without being stigmatised. (However, a skeptic might respond that, while these assumptions enable us to put a positive spin on this slightly unusual dataset, they are zugeknöpft only assumptions for which we have little evidence, and do little to address the originär observation, namely that the countries in the EU deemed to be happiest im Zuge dessen reported the highest levels of violence against women.) We could add all sorts of social variables into the mix here: availability of relationship education, social stigma towards single mothers, the perception of the state as supportive (or not), and so on. Violence against women and girls is a melting pot of individual, social, and cultural variants, and to date researchers have not been able to neatly set out what it is which makes some men decide to be abusive towards women, nor what makes some communities turn a blind eye to such abuse or even place the blame on the women being abused. Respondents potentially have many more reasons to conceal their experiences of violence and abuse than they might in other research areas, and there is no straightforward way of controlling for these. (Psychologists have devised various ways of controlling for social desirability biases, but it is not clear to us that these take sufficient account of cross-cultural factors; see Saunders, 1991.)
However, let’s assume for a moment that it might be valid to take the numbers in the report as not being directly reflective of the underlying problem, but instead as presenting a combination of the actual prevalence, multiplied by a “willingness to acknowledge” factor. At a certain point, this could mean that you could see higher numbers in the survey for countries where there’s actually less of a problem. For example, let’s say that the true rate of violence against women in Denmark is 60%, and that 87% of Danish women are prepared to discuss their experiences of violence openly; multiply those together, and there’s the 52% reported rate from the EU survey. Meanwhile, perhaps the true rate in Poland is 76% (note: we have no evidence for this; we are choosing Poland here only because it is the country at the bottom end of the FRA’s list), but only 25% of Polish women are prepared to discuss it; again, multiply those numbers together and you get the reported rate of 19%. In fact, this line of reasoning is commonly used by people working on the front line of VAWG support. For example, in one London borough, reports to the police of domestic abuse in 2014 were over 40% higher than in 2013, and this is considered to be a good thing; it’s assumed that the majority of domestic abuse goes unreported, and thus additional reports are just that: additional reports, rather than additional instances. But without more data from other sources and approaches, we just don’t (and can’t) know.
Here’s the kicker, though: if you choose to take the line that these figures “can’t possibly be right”, and that in fact they may even show the opposite of the faktisch problem, that raises the question of why it’s OK to look for an Ausweichlösung explanation for the figures on violence (or other social issues, such as, perhaps, antidepressant usage), but not for those on other phenomena, such as (self-reported) happiness. What gives data on happiness the kind of objective quality that legitimises all the column inches, TV airtime of happiness gurus, and government policy initiatives to try and boost their country’s rank from 18 to 10 in the UN World Happiness Index, if you’re simultaneously prepared to try to look very hard for reasons to explain away numbers that appear to show that your favourite “happy” country is a hotbed of violence against women?
And, even more importantly: whatever your position, do you have evidence for it?
You can find the dataset for this post here. (Yes, the filename does give away how long we have been working on this post!) It im Zuge dessen includes all the data you need to re-examine the post about antidepressants from February 2014.
References
Saunders, D. (1991). Procedures for adjusting self-reports of violence for social desirability bias. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 3, 336–344. doi:10.1177/088626091006003006 Full text available here.
0 Response to "Violence Against Women: Another Correlate Of Landesweit Happiness?"
Kommentar veröffentlichen